The Overlooked Observation That Keeps Us Coming Back
Bluma Zeigarnik’s framework for designing focus and closure.

If you love this newsletter and want to support the work that supports you, consider upgrading today! Readers keep this work alive!

BUSY THANKS YOU!!
The Overlooked Observation That Keeps Us Coming Back
I know the sag of the unfinished poem. And I know the release of the poem that is finished.
As I've gotten older, I've noticed how many known things I forget. The names of people's spouses, book titles (and often, their content). It's an unsettling feature of aging, but the more I think about what I've forgotten, the more I'm reminded of an undersung cognitive phenomenon that might explain the reason better than aging does.
The Zeigarnik Effect focuses on what we're prone to remember and why—and it's not what you'd expect—although it makes complete sense.
Let’s begin in Berlin.
It's 1926, and Bluma Zeigarnik is in a restaurant sharing a meal with her mentor Kurt Lewin—one of the founding figures of modern social psychology—and possibly other students (accounts differ). She's a graduate student from Russia, studying psychology. (She will later become a clinical pathopsychologist, a field she helped pioneer and develop.)

The waiter has nothing to write with, and Professor Lewin observes his ability to remember and track their complex orders. He recalls who ordered what, and who hadn't yet paid. Yet, what struck him was how much the waiter forgot once the check was settled and the plates cleared. The waiter seemed unable to recall not just details of their orders, but the patrons themselves.
After sharing these observations, Zeigarnik was intrigued enough to pursue a field of inquiry.
The Experiments
Building upon Lewin's emerging field theory, which emphasized the dynamic nature of human motivation, psychological environment—or "life space"— and the tension systems within the mind, Zeigarnik conducted a series of experiments to test whether interrupted tasks were actually more memorable than completed ones.
She gathered 164 people—children, students, even their teachers—and handed them a series of 18 to 22 small tasks. Some were puzzles, some arithmetic, some hands-on projects like building little cardboard boxes. Half the time she allowed them to finish; half the time she cut them off, just as they were getting somewhere.
Later, when she cleared the table and asked them to recall what they’d been working on, the results were startlingly consistent. Unfinished work stuck. In her first set of experiments, 26 of 32 people remembered the interrupted tasks best; in her replication with another 15 subjects, the pattern repeated almost exactly. Even in larger groups the story held: of 47 adults, 37 recalled the interrupted tasks most vividly, and among 45 schoolchildren, 36 did the same. On average, the uncompleted tasks were remembered nearly twice as well as the finished ones.
What we leave undone, she discovered, has a peculiar hold on the mind.

The Theory
Her theory was this: Incomplete tasks create a state of mental tension which persists until the task is completed. The motivation caused by this tension makes us better able to remember incomplete tasks.
Complete something? The mental tension dissolves, and the details fade (hence, my forgetting things! Maybe the logic doesn't hold up, but I feel like names I've known—and haven't "used" for a long time—might be "marked" as completed somehow).
At the time, her findings challenged prevailing theories of memory, which suggested that repetition was the key to memorability. Instead, Zeigarnik showed that the state of incompletion itself played a crucial role in how well information was retained.
The Zeigarnik Effect isn't about how strong your memory is—it's about how your brain prioritizes information. Incomplete tasks end up staying active in your awareness, irrespective of how good your memory is. Cognitive tension, which refers to a kind of psychological pressure that builds up when a task is started but left incomplete, is at the center of the Zeigarnik Effect.
Cognitive psychologists later built on this idea by introducing the concept of task-related cognitive load. This is the idea that incomplete goals take up working memory space. As Zeigarnik noted, it's not just memory that's affected. Focus, mood, and sleep are also affected. When left unmanaged, a pile-up of incomplete tasks can feel like an invisible burden.
Think about your own mental state right now. How many browser tabs do you have open? How many projects are sitting half-finished on your desktop, on your desk, in your mind? How many conversations ended mid-thought because someone got distracted by their phone?
We're living in an age of perpetual incompletion, and it's exhausting. Every unfinished email, every half-read article, every project you started but didn't finish, is creating one of those mental tensions Zeigarnik identified.
No wonder we feel mentally scattered and overwhelmed.
But what if we could turn this against itself? What if we could use our brain's obsession with unfinished business to actually get more done?

Louise Bourgeois
Below are five ways to harness the Zeigarnik Effect.
This piece continues, but you’re reading the free version of the How to Live newsletter. To read the rest and discover how to harness the Zeignarnik Effect to your advantage, please upgrade to paid.
Join How to Live
For people who live in their heads, feel more than they show, and want a language for both.
Already a paying subscriber? Sign In.
What you’ll receive as a subscriber::
- • Every new essay, the moment it’s published
- • Full access to the complete archive—150+ posts and counting
- • Bonus pieces and experiments-in-progress, shared occasionally
- • Invitations to seasonal, in-person gatherings
- • A direct line to me (annual subscribers): personal replies and tailored recommendations
- • 15% off all workshops and live events
Reply